Thursday, December 23, 2021

Happy Birthday Jerry Koosman!!

 


Koosman was underrated IMO. Toiled somewhat in the shadow of Seaver and some of the other phenoms that came through Shea Stadium. That 53.7 WAR doesn't lie. 

The leg drive demonstrated in the picture above almost looks like a LH version of Tom Seaver. No wonder those Miracle Mets were tough. 

from Stathead: 

Thursday, December 23

Born This Day

1942: Jerry Koosman: 53.7 WAR (turns 79)

1978: Victor Martinez: 32.0 WAR (turns 43)

1980: Cody Ross: 13.5 WAR (turns 41)

1983: Hanley Ramirez: 38.0 WAR (turns 38)

1988: Roberto Perez: 7.5 WAR (turns 33)

Wednesday, December 22, 2021

In Praise of Premortems (not postmortems)

from Farnam Street:

https://fs.blog/kahneman-better-decisions/ 

Daniel Kahneman’s Favorite Approach For Making Better Decisions

Bob Sutton’s book, Scaling Up Excellence: Getting to More Without Settling for Less, contains an interesting section towards the end on looking back from the future, which talks about “a mind trick that goads and guides people to act on what they know and, in turn, amplifies their odds of success.”

We build on Nobel winner Daniel Kahneman’s favorite approach for making better decisions. This may sound weird, but it’s a form of imaginary time travel.

It’s called the premortem. And, while it may be Kahneman’s favorite, he didn’t come up with it. A fellow by the name of Gary Klein invented the premortem technique.

A premortem works something like this. When you’re on the verge of making a decision, not just any decision but a big decision, you call a meeting. At the meeting, you ask each member of your team to imagine that it’s a year later.

Split them into two groups. Have one group imagine that the effort was an unmitigated disaster. Have the other pretend it was a roaring success. Ask each member to work independently and generate reasons, or better yet, write a story, about why the success or failure occurred. Instruct them to be as detailed as possible, and, as Klein emphasizes, to identify causes that they wouldn’t usually mention “for fear of being impolite.” Next, have each person in the “failure” group read their list or story aloud, and record and collate the reasons. Repeat this process with the “success” group. Finally use the reasons from both groups to strengthen your … plan. If you uncover overwhelming and impassible roadblocks, then go back to the drawing board.

Premortems encourage people to use “prospective hindsight,” or, more accurately, to talk in “future perfect tense.” Instead of thinking, “we will devote the next six months to implementing a new HR software initiative,” for example, we travel to the future and think, “we have devoted six months to implementing a new HR software package.”

You imagine that a concrete success or failure has occurred and look “back from the future” to tell a story about the causes.

[…]

Pretending that a success or failure has already occurred—and looking back and inventing the details of why it happened—seems almost absurdly simple. Yet renowned scholars including Kahneman, Klein, and Karl Weick supply compelling logic and evidence that this approach generates better decisions, predictions, and plans. Their work suggests several reasons why. …

1. This approach helps people overcome blind spots

As … upcoming events become more distant, people develop more grandiose and vague plans and overlook the nitty-gritty daily details required to achieve their long-term goals.

2. This approach helps people bridge short-term and long-term thinking

Weick argues that this shift is effective, in part, because it is far easier to imagine the detailed causes of a single outcome than to imagine multiple outcomes and try to explain why each may have occurred. Beyond that, analyzing a single event as if it has already occurred rather than pretending it might occur makes it seem more concrete and likely to actually happen, which motivates people to devote more attention to explaining it.

3. Looking back dampens excessive optimism

As Kahneman and other researchers show, most people overestimate the chances that good things will happen to them and underestimate the odds that they will face failures, delays, and setbacks. Kahneman adds that “in general, organizations really don’t like pessimists” and that when naysayers raise risks and drawbacks, they are viewed as “almost disloyal.”

Max Bazerman, a Harvard professor, believes that we’re less prone to irrational optimism when we predict the fate of projects that are not our own. For example, when it comes to friends’ home renovation projects, most people estimate the costs will run 25 to 50 percent over budget. When it comes to our projects; however, they will be “completed on time and near the project costs.”

4. A premortem challenges the illusion of consensus

Most times, not everyone on a team agrees with the course of action. Even when you have enough cognitive diversity in the room, people still keep their mouths shut because people in power tend to reward people who agree with them while punishing those who dare to speak up with a dissenting view.

The resulting corrosive conformity is evident when people don’t raise private doubts, known risks, and inconvenient facts. In contrast, as Klein explains, a premortem can create a competition where members feel accountable for raising obstacles that others haven’t. “The whole dynamic changes from trying to avoid anything that might disrupt harmony to trying to surface potential problems.”

11 Rules for Critical Thinking




from Farnam Street:

https://fs.blog/11-rules-for-critical-thinking/

11 Rules for Critical Thinking

A fantastic list of 11 rules from some of history’s greatest minds. These are Prospero’s Precepts and they are found in AKA Shakespeare: A Scientific Approach to the Authorship Question:


  • All beliefs in whatever realm are theories at some level. (Stephen Schneider)
  • Do not condemn the judgment of another because it differs from your own. You may both be wrong. (Dandemis)
  • Read not to contradict and confute; nor to believe and take for granted; nor to find talk and discourse; but to weigh and consider. (Francis Bacon)
  • Never fall in love with your hypothesis. (Peter Medawar)
  • It is a capital mistake to theorize before one has data. Insensibly one begins to twist facts to suit theories instead of theories to suit facts. (Arthur Conan Doyle)
  • A theory should not attempt to explain all the facts, because some of the facts are wrong. (Francis Crick)
  • The thing that doesn’t fit is the thing that is most interesting. (Richard Feynman)
  • To kill an error is as good a service as, and sometimes even better than, the establishing of a new truth or fact. (Charles Darwin)
  • It ain’t what you don’t know that gets you into trouble. It’s what you know for sure that just ain’t so. (Mark Twain)
  • Ignorance is preferable to error; and he is less remote from the truth who believes nothing, than he who believes what is wrong. (Thomas Jefferson)
  • All truth passes through three stages. First, it is ridiculed, second, it is violently opposed, and third, it is accepted as self-evident. (Arthur Schopenhauer)

P.S. - 

Insight

“The willingness to accept responsibility for one's own life is the source from which self-respect springs.”

— Joan Didion

Thursday, December 16, 2021

Who is Dick Lundy?

Dear Coaches:

Back in February 2021, our SABR Chapter tried to organize an essay contest to benefit Dick Lundy,

  1. ·         a former Negro League Baseball star,
  2. ·         citizen of Jacksonville, born and raised where FSCJ’s downtown location now       stands,
  3. ·         an alum of Edward Waters University (back when it was functioning as a high school)
  4. ·         and Bethune-Cookman when it was Cookman Institute.

Lundy will be a candidate to join Lloyd in the baseball Hall of Fame if they take another shot at adding ex-Negro League players in 2021. He is reported to have missed election the last time the Hall of Fame addressed Negro Leaguers in 2006. Lundy was not only a great ball player, but a great mentor, manager and a person who worked with Hall of Famer Effa Manley to try and salvage Negro league baseball as an entity after they were raided of talent following the Jackie Robinson signing.

We would like to elevate his profile locally and nationally through different initiatives to assist his candidacy. I have been in contact with next of kin and they feel like it is too late to affect this kind of change. Lundy is a deserving Hall of Famer on and off the field, statistically and subjectively he should be the next guy in.

I am afraid that locally, we have done too little to help, and I communicated that to a frustrated family. Please, please, please help me convince them that it is not too late. Nationally, the candidacy is on solid ground, but there are candidates whose local PR machine is greater than ours. We can change that, even now.

I wish I had essays from students to demonstrate to the family the potential of this project. They have been beaten down by the past. Now we can be involved in helping change baseball history. What could be better than that? Even a one-page essay from students would help, I dare say, especially from HBCU’s where he attended. This is a great diversity, equality and inclusion project for students. He and others were rendered invisible by Jim Crow, we cannot allow that crime to be repeated.

Please help, time is of the essence and is now more an enemy than a friend to our effort. Thank you for your consideration. I will help in any way I can.

Sincerely,


Charles Slavik

(813) 335-8678

Founder of John Henry “Pop” Lloyd SABR Chapter   

https://twitter.com/theslav1959

https://twitter.com/Floridasabr

 

Research Resources for Students:

http://sabr.org/bioproj/person/dick-lundy/

https://www.seamheads.com/NegroLgs/player.php?playerID=lundy01dic

http://www.banishedtothepen.com/who-is-dick-lundy/

http://www.cnlbr.org/Portals/0/Hero/Richard_Dick_Lundy.pdf

 

What Can Hitters Actually See Out of a Pitcher’s Hand? | FanGraphs Baseball



Cool stuff from FanGraphs!!

from FanGraphs.com

What Can Hitters Actually See Out of a Pitcher's Hand?

We've all seen those swings so terrible that a batter can't help but smile. Swings like this one from Brandon Phillips last year.
Phillips, of course, isn't the only victim of this sort of thing. He's been a league-average major-league hitter for a decade, which is a substantial accomplishment. But even accomplished hitters can look bad, can get it very wrong.
Were Phillips batting not for a last-place club but one contending for the postseason, we might gnash our teeth. Couldn't he see that was a slider? What was he thinking? What was he looking at?
The answer to that last question, turns out, is way more complicated than it seems. Phillips clearly should have laid off a breaking ball that failed to reach the plate. He clearly has done that — otherwise, he wouldn't have had a major-league career. So what happened? What did he see? Or not see? Ask hitters and experts that question, and the answers are vague, conflicting, and sometimes just strange.
There are those who are sure they see spin. "I'm just trying to pick up spin," said San Francisco infielder Joe Panik of what he's looking for out of the pitcher's hand. "I see spin on breaking balls," agreed teammate Brandon Belt. "I see spin pretty well," said Zack Greinke, this decade's best-hitting pitcher.
Here's the strange thing about seeing spin, though: there's evidence that the part of the brain that batters employ when a pitch is released isn't the part of the brain that sees motion. It's the part of the brain used for detail and color.
No problem. Imagine a flashing red/white mess — you can extrapolate from the color what spin is coming. It's basically what Preston Wilson said on the MLB Network, when he said he could see if the ball was "more red or more white" and that helped him know what pitch it was. That appearance inspired this whole post.
Greinke thought that was a real possibility. "It's the color you see, and the color will be moving in a different direction, and you know what that means," he said. It's funny, though, because people will swear it's not that way. Boston's Chris Young was pretty sure: "I see spin. I don't see color. I don't see red," he said before a game with the Blue Jays. He thought for a second, though. "Maybe I do and I don't think I do."
Whatever the neural mechanism, most batters to whom I spoke claimed to see spin of some sort. Could they see spin rate? Today's stat du jour is most often though of as an innate statistic that judges pitch quality, but spin plus arm slot equals movement, and we've always had movement. Could spin rate be actually about deception?
Rich Hill would say yes, as suggested by his comments about "mirroring" his high-vertical-spin curve with his high-vertical-spin fastball, in the opposite direction. Responses from hitters were mixed, however. "You can't see the speed of spin," thought Young. "The changeup is going to spin slower, I assume, but I can't see the speed of the spin." Jason Ochart, Head of Driveline Baseball Hitting Research and Development, said his hitters report that "with changeups (and, in particular, split fingers), they see a lack of spin."
Greinke agreed about spin as deception, calling back to his experience as a hitter facing different changeups: "I couldn't see [Johnny] Cueto's change, as well as other pitchers'. Some guys have really tight spin on their changeup and it's hard to see. Low-spin, you can probably see the spin, and it moves more. [Kris] Medlen's, I couldn't see the spin at all."
There has to be a decent amount of instinct and programmed reaction going on in these responses, though, because there's no time to think. "Think, and this game will grasp you and take you away," said Atlanta rookie Dansby Swanson.
A recent Washington Post report brought out the science behind that eloquent missive from Swanson. The part of your brain working on object recognition works with the part of your brain that controls your body, and the brain actually reduces blood flow to the frontal cortex, the part of your brain that you use when you make decisions that require some kinds of reflection.

Clayman79a

You don't have time to deliberate! "You have no time to react," said Baltimore's Manny Machado, and he was right, if not literally correct. The great Wall Street Journal infographic below details how a batter essentially has three blinks of an eye before the ball crosses the plate — and just over one blink of an eye before he has to stop or continue his swing. That's 225 milliseconds!

baseball-reaction-times

Still, there are things that batters swear they can see. Most felt they could see if the ball "popped out" of the pitcher's hand. Coach Ochart felt that was one of the most oft-used pieces of information: "Most commonly I hear that hitters can see the ball "pop upwards (relatively speaking)" out of the hand on breaking balls." Greinke, Swanson, and Young agreed, but Belt thought those "poppers" were a specific brand of curveball: "Some guys, it'll pop up, but that's just the get me over for a strike," he thought.

Watching a Blake Snell start, I thought maybe I was seeing him "wrap" his curveball, meaning that he was cocking his wrist hard, or supinating heavily in his grip, in order to get more movement on the pitch. That's fine, and it can lead to more movement as Trevor Bauer found, but it might also make the curve more obvious to the hitter, as some scouts have suggested to me.

Can hitters see fingers? If so, they'd be able to see those wrapped fingers. But Snell didn't think so. No hitter reported being able to see fingers on the release, but I hardly talked to all hitters ever. "It's really hard to see fingers on top," reported Panik. Steve Carter, Project Prospect scout and hitting coach, thought that some of his hitters could see them.

Take a look at a major-league pitcher — Caleb Cotham — releasing a four-seamer and a curve, overlaid on each other. Driveline Baseball used a high-speed camera to capture these images at the same point, and, well, seems pretty tough to see those fingers, at least to my eyes. Maybe you can see the different release point?

Here's something, though: I'm not a major-league hitter. Major-league hitters are very different from the public in general — and, specifically, when it comes to their vision. One study found that they have significantly better "mean visual acuity, distance steroacuity, and contrast sensitivity." Some of these guys can see fingers.

Interestingly, this is true even of non-professional baseball players. Another study compared youth baseball and softball players to young people who didn't play ball, and found that, even in the youth ranks, ballplayers had better visual skills.

Obviously it's probably a selection thing — if you can't see the ball well, you don't play baseball maybe — but there also seems to be a learned component to this whole thing. Here's Swanson, on what he thinks his brain is doing: "If you've faced a guy a couple times, get at-bats against them and then you get an idea of what they want to do, and you sort of visualize before the pitch comes, it's almost anticipatory."

Swanson's point brings us to another consideration: focusing too hard on the moment of release ignores the fact that this is a process that begins as soon as the pitcher begins his delivery.

Jim Lyons, Professor in the Department of Kinesiology at McMaster University, agreed that the central vision (cones) most used by players are sensitive to color and detail but not motion, and that the peripheral visual system (rods) are motion sensitive. But he said we can be accurate at estimating the velocity of an approach object "like a baseball or a fist" because of "retinal expansion — the image of an object on the retina, as it gets closer, expands." He calls the visual information "continuous."

That's important for spotting a fastball, which might be the most important thing a batter can do. For Machado, it's the only thing he can do. "I don't pick any of that stuff up," he laughed. "I see fastball, I swing. You know how fast the pitcher can throw, so you can see it coming out of his hand. If it's coming in hot, I'll swing at it. I can't tell out of the hand if it's going to be a slider or a changeup."

He's not alone. Oakland's Khris Davis could see some spin, but focused more on targeting fastballs and spotting location. "You can see some location early, too," he said, pointing out that the angle of the hand and the ball coming out could tell him if the ball would be up or down. "I like to look for release location," he added. "Lots of guys have different release points. Some hide it better than others."

So there's the other thing that's important about the fluidity of the moment: there are hints about what's coming out of the pitchers' hand that batters can spot in the delivery. Coach Carter echoed Davis ("You can generally see direction of the pitch, based on wrist and arm angle as it 'comes through'") and also talked about curveball deliveries being different ("forearm lay back"). Greinke focused on changeups, but talked about delivery cues: "Some pitchers will drop down a little, or slow their hand."

This stuff may be more important than what happens at the actual release point. A famous study of cricket bowlers isolated experience, delivery cues, and information after release to find which were the most important aspects. The first two were very important. The last? "Additional ball flight information provided no more advantage to this discrimination ability" — i.e., information after release wasn't very important.

Hold on. The release-point information isn't important? Maybe. Batters have to access the "swing" motor program before the pitch is released, really, and so there's little time to change that, Professor Lyons pointed out. You can really just halt it.

But what about this, I asked Lyons? This swing was started, not halted, and yet somehow Vladimir Guerrero did this.

Didn't he adjust to the flight of the ball? Post-release information? "I would argue that the swing path hasn't really been altered from the original motor program," responded Lyons. "Same action being executed as was planned on the basis of early perceptual information, but it's gone beyond the point of checking the swing (i.e., issuing the stop command) and hoping for the best." In other words, Vlad thought the ball would be low, the ball was low, and then… "the ball hit the bat (rather than the other way around)" as Lyons put it.

As much as hitters don't like to think too much — Belt said "When I get myself in trouble, it's when I guess and anticipate too much" — it looks like anticipating the pitch is huge.

Recently, I was at Pitch Talks in Toronto, talking backstage with Pete Abraham of the Boston Globe about baseball, and we ended up talking about David Ortiz, as one does. We first talked about how Ortiz spent a replay delay talking about how Matt Andriese would throw him a first-pitch curveball. The pitcher did, and Ortiz homered.

If that's not a sufficiently impressive display of anticipation — one that might, for instance, explain some of the slugger's late-career excellence — there's more. Abraham mentioned something about Xander Bogaerts enjoying the spot in the batting order ahead of David Ortiz. When I raised an eyebrow, he explained. It seems that Ortiz will often follow along in Bogaerts' at-bats and try to alert him to when a certain pitch is coming with chirps and whistles from the on-deck circle. What anticipation!

It's hard for me to completely discount the value of information received at the moment of release, but it's also interesting how many hitters had no idea what happened at that moment.

Some excerpts from the players themselves…
MVP candidate Mookie Betts:
"I don't really know what I see. I see this white thing coming, and I somehow try to read it. I try to look at a window where his slot is and try to pick it up as fast as possible. You try to read spin, but the way I do it, I have no idea."
Edwin Encarnacion:
"I just see the ball, it's white or red, but I try not to think, and I don't want to start thinking."
Panik:
"It's hard to talk about."
Astros hitting coach Dave Hudgens:
"I know when a hitter is locked in, or in the zone, nothing is in their mind and everything slows down."
Coach Ochart:
"Some of the best hitters I've ever coached will "black out" when they hit. Very interesting. I'll ask them what pitch they hit, or what location the pitch was, and they won't remember. 'Idk coach, I just see ball, hit ball.'"
This experience of "blacking out" probably speaks to the lack of frontal-cortex activity during the moment — hitters aren't using the part of the brain that's best for mapping out complicated, deliberate decisions, so maybe it's not surprising they bypass the frontal cortex and use deeper, darker parts of the brain. Some have never thought about this action using the speaking part of their brain — "Nobody's ever asked me that question," said Betts. When the major work is unlocking motor programs, or chunks, how do you explain that?

That doesn't mean we can't try to put words to it using the best research and player quotes at our disposal. Call this a conclusion, but not the type of conclusion you put on the end of an academic paper. Just imagine a big, blinking neon sign alternating between the words "maybe," "probably," and "could be" above this paragraph.

It looks like anticipating the pitch is a benefit. Combining that idea of the pitcher's tendencies with pre-release delivery information might be more important than the information at release. At release, hitters might access motor programs (primed by anticipation) for each type of pitch, and then use the release information to stop their swing if it doesn't line up with what they expected. That info on release is probably mostly velocity, with some movement extrapolated from changes in color (and then called "spin" in the parlance of our times), but every hitter reported that sensitivity to that information came and went. Once the ball is in flight, if they're wrong, it could be a matter of luck (and maybe a little geometry — think of the flat swing) if they swing and still manage to hit the ball.

All of this has ramifications for coaching! Professor Lyons suggested doing a study to see what hitters could do with a ball with white seams, which seems like a great idea. Brian Dozier even complained that current balls all look "brown" because of the mud — perhaps spin and seam movement is over-reported and overemphasized. Coach Ochart seemed to suggest that this sort of research is planned for Driveline Hitting, so we can look forward to more results soon, maybe.

If that aspect of coaching is overrated, we might have already had clues in this direction. Russell Carleton found evidence that hitting coaches mostly affect the patience or passivity of their teams, and that might be the rudder here. How early do you get your hitters going, and how quickly can you get your hitters to stop swinging, while possibly helping your batters align their swings geometrically with the outcomes that fit their athletic skill sets: that seems to be the work of a hitting coach. Perhaps you can help them "build a mental database" about pitchers, as Coach Carter refers to it. That should help with anticipation, after all. But then even he says they have to "go off instinct" at the plate.
Certainly coaches shouldn't get mad at the hitter with whom they're working. The whole thing — hitting 2400 rpm at 96 mph with a blink of an eye of time to figure it out — seems pretty impossible, after all.


Sent from my iPhone

Giants Top Minor League Prospects

  • 1. Joey Bart 6-2, 215 C Power arm and a power bat, playing a premium defensive position. Good catch and throw skills.
  • 2. Heliot Ramos 6-2, 185 OF Potential high-ceiling player the Giants have been looking for. Great bat speed, early returns were impressive.
  • 3. Chris Shaw 6-3. 230 1B Lefty power bat, limited defensively to 1B, Matt Adams comp?
  • 4. Tyler Beede 6-4, 215 RHP from Vanderbilt projects as top of the rotation starter when he works out his command/control issues. When he misses, he misses by a bunch.
  • 5. Stephen Duggar 6-1, 170 CF Another toolsy, under-achieving OF in the Gary Brown mold, hoping for better results.
  • 6. Sandro Fabian 6-0, 180 OF Dominican signee from 2014, shows some pop in his bat. Below average arm and lack of speed should push him towards LF.
  • 7. Aramis Garcia 6-2, 220 C from Florida INTL projects as a good bat behind the dish with enough defensive skill to play there long-term
  • 8. Heath Quinn 6-2, 190 OF Strong hitter, makes contact with improving approach at the plate. Returns from hamate bone injury.
  • 9. Garrett Williams 6-1, 205 LHP Former Oklahoma standout, Giants prototype, low-ceiling, high-floor prospect.
  • 10. Shaun Anderson 6-4, 225 RHP Large frame, 3.36 K/BB rate. Can start or relieve
  • 11. Jacob Gonzalez 6-3, 190 3B Good pedigree, impressive bat for HS prospect.
  • 12. Seth Corry 6-2 195 LHP Highly regard HS pick. Was mentioned as possible chip in high profile trades.
  • 13. C.J. Hinojosa 5-10, 175 SS Scrappy IF prospect in the mold of Kelby Tomlinson, just gets it done.
  • 14. Garett Cave 6-4, 200 RHP He misses a lot of bats and at times, the plate. 13 K/9 an 5 B/9. Wild thing.

2019 MLB Draft - Top HS Draft Prospects

  • 1. Bobby Witt, Jr. 6-1,185 SS Colleyville Heritage HS (TX) Oklahoma commit. Outstanding defensive SS who can hit. 6.4 speed in 60 yd. Touched 97 on mound. Son of former major leaguer. Five tool potential.
  • 2. Riley Greene 6-2, 190 OF Haggerty HS (FL) Florida commit.Best HS hitting prospect. LH bat with good eye, plate discipline and developing power.
  • 3. C.J. Abrams 6-2, 180 SS Blessed Trinity HS (GA) High-ceiling athlete. 70 speed with plus arm. Hitting needs to develop as he matures. Alabama commit.
  • 4. Reece Hinds 6-4, 210 SS Niceville HS (FL) Power bat, committed to LSU. Plus arm, solid enough bat to move to 3B down the road. 98MPH arm.
  • 5. Daniel Espino 6-3, 200 RHP Georgia Premier Academy (GA) LSU commit. Touches 98 on FB with wipe out SL.

2019 MLB Draft - Top College Draft Prospects

  • 1. Adley Rutschman C Oregon State Plus defender with great arm. Excellent receiver plus a switch hitter with some pop in the bat.
  • 2. Shea Langliers C Baylor Excelent throw and catch skills with good pop time. Quick bat, uses all fields approach with some pop.
  • 3. Zack Thompson 6-2 LHP Kentucky Missed time with an elbow issue. FB up to 95 with plenty of secondary stuff.
  • 4. Matt Wallner 6-5 OF Southern Miss Run producing bat plus mid to upper 90's FB closer. Power bat from the left side, athletic for size.
  • 5. Nick Lodolo LHP TCU Tall LHP, 95MPH FB and solid breaking stuff.